Research Article |
Corresponding author: Luis Espinasa ( luis.espinasa@marist.edu ) Academic editor: Eleonora Trajano
© 2022 Luis Espinasa, Emily Collins, C. Patricia Ornelas García, Sylvie Rétaux, Nicolas Rohner, Jennifer Rutkowski.
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Citation:
Espinasa L, Collins E, Ornelas García CP, Rétaux S, Rohner N, Rutkowski J (2022) Divergent evolutionary pathways for aggression and territoriality in Astyanax cavefish. Subterranean Biology 43: 169-183. https://doi.org/10.3897/subtbiol.73.79318
|
The surface morph of the Mexican tetra fish (Astyanax mexicanus) exhibits strong territoriality behavior and high levels of aggression. In contrast, the eyeless cave-adapted morph from Sierra de El Abra, México, rarely are aggressive and have totally lost the territorial behavior. These behaviors are part of what has been called the cavefish behavioral syndrome. Here, we report that several Astyanax cave populations of Sierra de Guatemala, unlike those reported for the Sierra de El Abra cave populations, display significant territoriality and aggression when confined into a reduced space. We discuss divergent evolutionary trajectories in terms of agonistic behavior for cavefish populations inhabiting different mountain ranges.
Abra, behavior, stygobyte, troglobite
Organisms in which variation in behavior can be linked to the environment and their evolutionary history are key to understanding how behavior evolve. The Mexican tetra Astyanax mexicanus has both an eyed surface morph and a blind cave-adapted morph. The cave-dwelling Astyanax are characterized by conspicuous morphological traits that have evolved in response to their subterranean environment. Among these traits, the most obvious are degenerate eyes and reduced pigmentation (
Astyanax
genus has emerged as a powerful model system for genotype-phenotype analysis because surface fish and cavefish morphs are interfertile and high-quality genome information is available (
Localities of cave populations A satellite photo of Sierra de Guatemala (Red) and Sierra de El Abra (Blue). Inset shows the area of México. Map data 2021 (C) Google B topographic map modified from Elliot (
Aggressive interactions and territoriality are correlated, with fish defending an individual space. Astyanax surface fish show territoriality that is dependent upon the aquarium space available. In small tanks less than 250 L, surface fish stop schooling and start defending territories (
The intense dominance-related aggressiveness in surface Astyanax is inversely correlated with the serotonin (5HT) levels in the hindbrain raphe nucleus (
Since populations in three mountain ranges (El Abra, Micos, and Guerrero) have independently converged in a reduction of aggression and territoriality, it has been suggested that the loss may be an adaptive change for cave Astyanax (
Cavefish and surface fish localities used in this study can be seen in figure 1. Quantitative data was gathered from fish within four days after being collected from the field in 2016 (Pachón cave collected on August 6th, Caballo Moro cave on May 18th; and Rascón surface fish on August 5th). Data was also gathered from Molino, Vásquez, Caballo Moro, Jineo, and Escondido cavefish, kept in P. Ornelas’ laboratory at IBUNAM, México, which had been collected on February 2016, four months prior to the study. Choy surface river and Tinaja cavefish originally bred in Borowsky’s laboratory and now kept in Espinasa’s laboratory were also analyzed. Fish kept in the laboratory have been under standard husbandry with a water filtration and aeration system. Fish have been fed about 3% of the fish’s bodyweight daily. Fish used in experiments were not fed on the 24 hrs before the experiments. Since the Caballo Moro cave holds a mixed population of eyed and eyeless fish, only completely eyeless Caballo Moro specimens were used (Fig.
Ramming and biting attempts were counted as attacks following
To study territoriality, four fish were left in a tank to acclimate overnight. The next morning, they were filmed with a DCRSR42 Sony Digital camera from above for three minutes. Since fish tend to scatter beyond their territories for a few moments after an attack, disrupting territorial patterns, the one-minute section of the film with fewer attacks was selected and examined in the laboratory to track the paths of each individual. For quantification, in the video the tank was divided into four quadrants. The track generated by the fish was analyzed and the number of quadrants not occupied throughout one minute by the individual fish were counted. The assumption was that territorial fish would stay in their territory and some quadrants would not be included in their paths, while non-territorial fish would swim throughout the tank. Samples analyzed were from Sierra de El Abra (Pachón n = 8 tests) and Sierra de Guatemala (Caballo Morro n = 8 and Molino n = 4). A Mann-Whitney U test was done to establish if the populations differed in how many quadrants were not occupied. Fish from other caves (Vásquez, Jineo, Escondido and Tinaja) were also observed qualitatively for signs of territoriality while in their host tanks at Ornelas’ laboratory, where they had been acclimatized for months.
Since territoriality in Astyanax cavefish has not been previously reported, most authors have never witnessed this behavior in action. For illustration purposes and to facilitate uniformity of criteria in future studies, a video was recorded of cavefish being aggressive and how they establish territories using the intruder essay. Two Caballo Moro specimens were placed into one tank and five into another tank to acclimate overnight. The next morning, they were filmed as described above. Afterwards, an “intruder” fish from the tank with five fish was transferred to the tank with the two fish and left for 20 min to acclimatize, after which, another three minutes were filmed. Again, the one-minute section of the film with fewer attacks was selected to track and mark the preferred territories of each fish. Afterwards, another intruder was added to obtain the swimming paths of four fish.
In the four fish assay (Fig.
On the contrary, Sierra de Guatemala cavefish populations (Molino cave fish x̄ = 16.0 +/- 11.0 SD, n = 18; Caballo Moro x̄ = 25.7 +/- 21.0, n = 17; Vásquez x̄ = 9.7 +/- 7.3, n = 9; and Jineo × = 30, n = 1) had aggression levels not significantly different from surface Choy population (P = 0.68–0.15), and even Caballo Moro cave population was not significantly different (P = 0.06) from the highly aggressive Rascón surface population. All these Sierra de Guatemala cavefish populations were significantly more aggressive (P = 0.002-0.001) than the Sierra de El Abra and Escondido cavefish.
Similar results were obtained in the two fish assay (Fig.
Examples of aggressive interactions of Sierra de Guatemala cavefish can be seen in https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=8IW7hgzZbWI.
Eyeless Caballo Moro specimen used for aggression and territoriality studies A body B head. While the Caballo Moro cave is inhabited by both the eyed and the eyeless morphs, only eyeless specimens were used in this study. The other populations used in this study; Molino, Jineo, Vásquez, Escondido, Pachón and Tinaja caves are inhabited exclusively by eyeless fish.
Number (with SD) of ramming and biting attempts in the four fish A and two fish B assays, for Sierra de El Abra (Pachón and Tinaja), Sierra de Guatemala (Escondido, Caballo Moro, Molino, and Vásquez) cave fish and surface fish (Choy and Rascón). With the exception of Escondido cave, Sierra de Guatemala cave fish were significantly more aggressive than Sierra de El Abra cave fish. In several comparison, Sierra de Guatemala cave populations were as aggressive and not significantly different than some of the surface fish. Tables below graphs show significance levels. Notice that Pachón, Tinaja and Escondido are significantly less aggressive than all of the rest.
Sierra de El Abra cavefish (i.e. Pachón and Tinaja) show no tendency to establish territories. Pachón individuals swam throughout the tank, preferentially following the edges, crossing paths with other individuals constantly (Fig.
Pachón cavefish had fewer quadrants not traversed in their paths (0.0 +/- 0.0 SD, n = 8 individual fish) in one minute than Caballo Moro (2.5 +/- 0.7, n = 8; P=.00094 Mann-Whitney U test) and Molino (1.25 +/- 1.5, n = 4; sample size too small to test with U test). In all tests, Pachón cavefish shared all sectors of the tank and all individual’s paths crossed all four quadrants of the tank. The average time spent in the single most used quadrant for Pachón cavefish was 47.9% (x̄ = 28.7+/- 8.0 seconds out of 60), while Caballo Moro cavefish spent 90.8% (x̄ = 54.5+/- 5.0 seconds out of 60) and Molino 75.4% (x̄ = 45.2+/- 13.8 seconds out of 60) of the time in a single quadrant or corner of the tank and all fish failed to enter between one to three of the other quadrants. This suggests that Sierra de Guatemala cavefish remained within a constrained area or territory, unlike Pachón fish. Fish from other Sierra de Guatemala caves (Vásquez and Jineo) appeared to establish territories at a slower pace than Caballo Moro and Molino fish. Therefore, only a qualitative observation was done in their host tanks at Ornelas’ laboratory where they had been acclimatized for months for signs of territoriality. In those conditions, cavefish were also seen swimming in paths clearly restricted to only certain areas of the tanks. Qualitative observations of Pachón, Tinaja and Escondido cavefish in their host tanks where they have also been kept for months failed to show equivalent swimming paths restricted to territories.
Examples of territoriality of Sierra de Guatemala cavefish can be seen in https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=8IW7hgzZbWI.
Territoriality from a top view of a tank. Black straight lines represent edges of the tanks (size 20cm X 40cm). Colored lines follow the swimming paths of four individuals over a 1 min period. A Thin lines follow four Pachón cavefish. B Thick lines follow four eyeless Caballo Moro cavefish. Yellow star signals an attack. Specimens used for both cavefish populations are eyeless. Notice that while all Pachón fish swim throughout the entirety of the tank and the paths of individual fish converge over the paths of other individuals, those of Caballo Moro are mostly restricted to a corner, and each individual holds a territory seldom crossed by another individual. However, when they do cross, it can result in an attack.
While most authors agree that aggression in Astyanax cavefish populations from Sierra de El Abra, Micos, (reviewed in
In the present study, cavefish were directly observed by authors of both the
But there is a second alternative that focuses on the variability in aggression levels among surface populations. Sierra de Guatemala cavefish are as aggressive as surface fish from Río Boquillas and Choy, but less aggressive than those from Rascón. Qualitative and quantitative observations in the fish kept at the UNAM laboratory suggest that Rascón fish are more aggressive than other surface populations from streams closer to Sierra de El Abra. While few live specimens are available, tanks with Rascón fish typically need to have reduced number of individuals so as to prevent them from killing each other. Rascón population is isolated from the rest of the surface populations by the 100 m high Tamul waterfall and its known to be genetically distinct by harboring a different mitochondrial haplotype from other surface populations (
Our study also confirmed that the absence of aggression in the three Molino individuals kept in France used in the
It has been suggested that agonistic behaviors in surface Astyanax rely on visual cues and that aggressiveness is no longer performed in darkness at all (
Our analyses of the swimming paths indicate that unlike Sierra de El Abra cavefish, most Sierra de Guatemala cavefish develop territories. Individuals appear to try to minimize contact with each other and the available space is subdivided. The borders of these territories are where most attacks are concentrated. Again, it is worth noting that the cave specimens in these assays were completely eyeless individuals and thus they could not use visual cues to generate a spatial mapping of the tank and their territories. Nonetheless, individuals adjusted their swimming paths to form new and stable territories without the help of visual cues.
Swimming paths of most Sierra de Guatemala cavefish kept inside the laboratory are drastically different from Sierra de El Abra cavefish. Pachón and Tinaja fish typically swim near the edges of the tank, following the entirety of the side and turning at the corners, thus going in circles around the whole tank. Aggressive Sierra de Guatemala fish that hold territories have swimming paths that do not follow the entirety of the side of the tank. Instead, they swim a few strokes and turn around before encountering any physical obstruction. Non-visual spatial mapping must allow them to turn at the frontier of their territories, even when the neighboring individual is distant. At high densities, swimming paths can even be restricted to reduced circling around in areas only slightly larger than the fish itself.
Based on studies of the Pachón and Micos cave, the surface fish, and F1-hybrids between surface and Pachón fish,
Several Astyanax cave populations of Sierra de Guatemala, unlike those reported for the Sierra de El Abra cave populations, display significant territoriality and aggression when confined into a reduced space. The Sierra de Guatemala cavefish may have achieved the regulation of adaptive feeding behaviors or the control of serotonergic networks through different evolutionary, genetic, and physiologic pathways than at Sierra de El Abra populations. Thus, the pleiotropic and side effects may be different such that aggression and territoriality were maintained.
Jae Wong helped process the videos. Support for a short sabbatical stay at CNRS in France to LE was provided by a VPAA grant from Marist College and by an ANR (Agence Nationale pour la Recherche) grant ASTYCO to SR. Work supported by an CONACYT, ECOS-NORD, exchange grant between SR and POG. We thank the support from the CONACYT project N191986 to POG. Zsuzsanna Sidlo provided comments on the manuscript. The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.